Academic Home

Thoughts on the Edge of Chaos

M.M.Taylor and R.A. Pigeau

 

1. Overview

2. Basic Ideas: Information and structure, Attractors and Repellors

3. Basic Ideas: Catastrophe

4. Structure and Chaos

5. Six Kinds of Replication

6. Categories and Logic

7. Surprise and importing structure

8. Replication

 

Catastrophe

The idea of catastrophe is another fundamental notion once fashionable in cognitive theory (for example, the whole issue of Behavioral Science, 1978, vol 23 No. 5 was devoted entirely to "Applications of catastrophe theory in the behavioral and life sciences").

To define a catastrophe there must be variables of two classes: call them "input" and "output," or "state" and "consequence." In a non-catastrophic relationship, each possible value of the input variables corresponds to a unique value for each output variable.

In a castrophic relationship, the output has more than one value for a given input over at least part of the input range. Moreover, there are limiting values of the input at which the output changes from multi-valued to single-valued, and at which at least two of the multiple values are distinct. If the input moves across a limiting value while the output is at a value different from the output on the single-valued part of the function, the output moves abruptly (catastrophically) to the new value

Fig 2. A function that shows a fold catastrophe. As the input data move up from zero, the output stays on the lower part of the curve until it abruptly switches to the upper curve. But when the data input goes down again, it does not return to the lower part of the curve until a lower value is reached than the one at which the output flipped on the way up. The function shows "hysteresis". If in some way the input and output are made to take values on the intermediate "reverse" part of the curve, the slightest disturbance will cause the output to flip to the outer branch in the direction of the disturbance. The outer branches are stable "attractors", the inner on a metastable "repellor".

 

Figures 1 and 2 represent one-dimensional input and output variables with, respectively, no catastrophe and a "fold" catastrophe. If a second input variable is added that controls a transition between these two states, it may generate a function known as a "cusp" catastrophe.

Fig 3. A cusp catastrophe generated by a modulator, or "stress" variable. At low stress, the output varies smoothly with the data input, but as the stress increases, the output must take on values from one or other of the outer branches of the embedded fold.

Fig 4. Example of cusp in the perception of three (possibly related) lines. With little stress, the perception is of three lines angled with respect to one another. When there is stress to produce a "perceptual decision" the perception is of either "A" or "H".

Cusp catastrophes will be shown to be important in learning, and to provide a route between the chaos of alertness on the one hand, and the stability of categoric perception or the precision of logical thought on the other.


Fig 5a. At an early stage of learning, there is no discrimination, although the learner can perceive differences along the data dimension, and the output may be stronger as the modulator input (stress) increases.

Fig 5b. As learning begins, the learner is able to make choices between the two categories of item, but does so only when the modulator input is strong, and a decision is forced by either context or task pressure.

Fig 5c. When the discrimination is well learned, the learner will tend to make the discrimination even with very little pressure to do so. The data will "automatically" be seen as representing one of the learned categories, rather than just as a variable data value.


The modulator input might be pressure from a teacher to make a decision as to which category is represented by the data,it might be local context (e.g. the "three-stick" figure is likely to represent a letter A or H if it is surrounded by other letters such as C*T or T*E), or it might be a "need to act" in some situation. Decisions are easier and occur with less modulator stress if the discrimination is well learned.

 

Top of Page
Previous pagenext page